Perspective is a powerful force in creating the impression of depth, even when other cues work against it. A luminous trapezoid viewed in a frontal plane, for example, when seen with one eye in a dark room will look like a rectangle at a slant. Even when such a figure is viewed with both eyes, which provides information from stereopsis, perspective strongly affects what is perceived, as Barbara Gillam, then at the Australian National University, and William Epstein and his associates at the University of Wisconsin have shown. Moreover, Epstein has demonstrated that continued binocular viewing of objects in the environment with a special lens in front of one eye that alters retinal disparity will result in an adaptive recalibration of retinal disparity. Ordinarily, retinal disparity and perspective yield congruent information, such as, for example, that a rectangle is slanted back from the frontal plane by 30 degrees. In the experiment, however, the two eyes are made to provide discrepant information: the perspective information that objects are in a frontal plane is at odds with the retinal disparity information that they are slanted backwards in depth. Apparently, the subjects learn to associate unconsciously a given disparity with some new depth, not the depth the disparity signified before, because, in a subsequent test for depth based on disparity using two luminous dots, the subjects saw depth under conditions of zero disparity and, conversely, no depth when some actual disparity was present. Thus, in only minutes, a pictorial cue such as linear perspective is sufficiently potent to reeducate the mind about stereopsis.